From an engineering standpoint, humans would presumably be far better off if evolution had supplemented our contextually driven memory with a way of systematically searching our inventory of memories. Just as a pollster's data are most accurate if taken from a representative cross section of a population, a human's beliefs would be soundest if they were based on a balanced set of evidence. But alas, evolution never discovered the statistician's notion of an unbiased sample.

Instead, we routinely take whatever memories are most recent or most easily remembered to be much more important than any other data. Consider, for example, an experience I had recently, driving across country and wondering at what time I'd arrive at the next motel. When traffic was moving well, I'd think to myself, 'Wow, I'm driving at 80 miles per hour on the interstate; I'll be there in an hour.' When traffic slowed due to construction, I'd say, 'Oh no, it'll take me two hours.' What I was almost comically unable to do was to take an average across two data points at the same time, and say, 'Sometimes the traffic moves well, sometimes it moves poorly. I anticipate a mixture of good and bad, so I bet it will take an hour and a half.'

Some of the world's most mundane but common interpersonal friction flows directly from the same failure to reflect on how well our samples represent reality. When we squabble with our spouse or our roommate about whose turn it is to wash the dishes, we probably (without realizing it) are better able to remember the previous times when we, ourself, took care of them (as compared to the times when our roommate or spouse did); after all, our memory is organized to focus primarily on our own experience. And we rarely compensate for that imbalance — so we come to believe we've done more work overall and perhaps end up in a self-righteous huff. Studies show that in virtually any collaborative enterprise, from taking care of a household to writing academic papers with colleagues, the sum of each individual's perceived contribution exceeds the total amount of work done. We cannot remember what other people did as well as we recall what we did ourselves — which leaves everybody (even shirkers!) feeling that others have taken advantage of them. Realizing the limits of our own data sampling might make us all a lot more generous.

Mental contamination is so potent that even entirely irrelevant information can lead us by the nose. In one pioneering experiment, the psychologists Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman spun a wheel of fortune, marked with the numbers 1-100, and then asked their subjects a question that had nothing to do with the outcome of spinning the wheel: what percentage of African countries are in the United Nations? Most participants didn't know for sure, so they had to estimate

— fair enough. But their estimates were considerably affected by the number on the wheel. When the wheel registered 10, a typical response to the UN question was 25 percent, whereas when the wheel came up at 65, a typical response was 45 percent.*

This phenomenon, which has come to be known as 'anchoring and adjustment,' occurs again and again. Try this one: Add 400 to the last three digits of your cell phone number. When you're done, answer the following question: in what year did Attila the Hun's rampage through Europe finally come to an end? The average guess of

*Nobody's ever been able to tell me whether the original question was meant to ask how many of the countries in Africa were in the UN, or how many of the countries in the UN were in Africa. But in a way, it doesn't matter: anchoring is strong enough to apply even when we don't know precisely what the question is.

people whose phone number, plus 400, yields a sum less than 600 was

A.D. 629, whereas the average guess of people whose phone number digits plus 400 came in between 1,200 and 1,399 was A.D. 979, 350 years later.*

What's going on here? Why should a phone number or a spin on a wheel of fortune influence a belief about history or the composition of the UN? During the process of anchoring and adjustment, people begin at some arbitrary starting point and keep moving until they find an answer they like. If the number 10 pops up on the wheel, people start by asking themselves, perhaps unconsciously, 'Is 10 a plausible answer to the UN question?' If not, they work their way up until they find a value (say, 25) that seems plausible. If 65 comes up, they may head in the opposite direction: 'Is 65 a plausible answer? How about 55?' The trouble is, anchoring at a single arbitrarily chosen point can steer us toward answers that are just barely plausible: starting low leads people to the lowest plausible answer, but starting high leads them to the highest plausible answer. Neither strategy directs people to what might be the most sensible response — one in the middle of the range of plausible answers. If you think that the correct answer is somewhere between 25 and 45, why say 25 or 45? You're probably better off guessing 35, but the psychology of anchoring means that people rarely do.f

Anchoring has gotten a considerable amount of attention in psychological literature, but it's by no means the only illustration of how beliefs and judgments can be contaminated by peripheral or even irrelevant information. To take another example, people who are asked to hold a pen between their teeth gently, without letting it

*When did Attila actually get routed? A.D. 451.

tlf you're aware of the process of anchoring and adjustment, you can see that why it is

that during a financial negotation it's generally better to make the opening bid than to

respond to it. This phenomenon also explains why, as one recent study showed, su

permarkets can sell more cans of soup with signs that say LIMIT 12 PER CUS

TOMER rather than LIMIT 4 PER CUSTOMER.

touch their lips, rate cartoons as more enjoyable than do people who hold a pen with pursed lips. Why should that be? You can get a hint if you try following these instructions while looking in a mirror: Hold a pen between your teeth 'gently, without letting it touch the lips.' Now look at the shape of your lips. You'll see that the corners are upturned, in the position of a smile. And thus, through the force of context-dependent memory, upturned lips tend to automatically lead to happy thoughts.

A similar line of experiments asked people to use their non-dominant hand (the left, for right-handed people) to write down names of celebrities as fast as they could while classifying them into categories (like, dont like, neutral). They had to do this while either

(1) pressing their dominant hand, palm down, against the top of a table or (2) pushing their dominant hand, palm upward, against the bottom of a table. Palms-up people listed more positive than negative names, while palms-down people produced more negative names than positive. Why? Palms-up people were positioned in a positive 'approach' posture while palms-down people were positioned in an 'avoid' posture. The data show that such subtle differences routinely affect our memories and, ultimately, our beliefs.

Another source of contamination is a kind of mental shortcut, the human tendency to believe that what is

Добавить отзыв
ВСЕ ОТЗЫВЫ О КНИГЕ В ИЗБРАННОЕ

0

Вы можете отметить интересные вам фрагменты текста, которые будут доступны по уникальной ссылке в адресной строке браузера.

Отметить Добавить цитату
×