cent. (2003–05 data in table under references).
The rising trend of cannabis potency is gradual, fairly unspectacular, and driven largely by the increased availability of domestic, intensively grown indoor herbal cannabis.
Mean potency (% THC) of cannabis products examined in the UK (Forensic Science Service, 1995–2002)
Year Sinsemilla %Resin %‘Traditional’ imported herbal %
1995
1996
1997
1998
1999
2000
2001
2002
2003
2004
2005
5.8
8.0
9.4
10.5
10.6
12.2
12.3
12.3
12.0
12.7
14.2
No dada
No dada
No dada
6.1
4.4
4.2
6.7
3.2
4.6
1.6
6.6
3.9
5.0
4.0
3.8
5.0
8.5
No dada
No dada
No dada
No dada
No dada
Mean THC content of cannabis products seized in the UK (Forensic Science Service, 1995–2002)
‘Twenty-five times stronger’, remember. Repeatedly, and on the front page.
If you were in the mood to quibble with the
There is, of course, exceptionally strong cannabis to be found in some parts of the British market today, but then there always has been. To get its scare figure, the
And this scare isn’t even new. In the mid-1980s, during Ronald Reagan’s ‘war on drugs’ and Zammo’s ‘Just say no’ campaign on
That’s not even a crystal in a plant pot. It’s impossible. It would require more THC to be present in the plant than the total volume of space taken up by the plant itself. It would require matter to be condensed into super- dense quark-gluon-plasma cannabis. For God’s sake don’t tell the
We are now ready to move on to some more interesting statistical issues, with another story from an emotive area, an article in
If you read the press release for the government survey on which the story is based, it reports ‘almost no change in patterns of drug use, drinking or smoking since 2000’. But this was a government press release, and journalists are paid to investigate: perhaps the press release was hiding something, to cover up for government failures. The
You can download the full document online. It’s a survey of 9,000 children, aged eleven to fifteen, in 305 schools. The three-page summary said, again, that there was no change in prevalence of drug use. If you look at the full report you will find the raw data tables: when asked whether they had used cocaine in the past year, 1 per cent said yes in 2004, and 2 per cent said yes in 2005.
So the newspapers were right: it doubled? No. Almost all the figures given were 1 per cent or 2 per cent. They’d all been rounded off. Civil servants are very helpful when you ring them up. The actual figures were 1.4 per cent for 2004, and 1.9 per cent for 2005, not 1 per cent and 2 per cent. So cocaine use hadn’t doubled at all. But people were still eager to defend this story: cocaine use, after all, had increased, yes?
No. What we now have is a relative risk increase of 35.7 per cent, or an absolute risk increase of 0.5 per cent. Using the real numbers, out of 9,000 kids we have about forty-five more saying ‘Yes’ to the question ‘Did you take cocaine in the past year?’
Presented with a small increase like this, you have to think: is it statistically significant? I did the maths, and the answer is yes, it is, in that you get a p-value of less than 0.05. What does ‘statistically significant’ mean? It’s just a way of expressing the likelihood that the result you got was attributable merely to chance. Sometimes you might throw ‘heads’ five times in a row, with a completely normal coin, especially if you kept tossing it for long enough. Imagine a jar of 980 blue marbles, and twenty red ones, all mixed up: every now and then – albeit rarely –