WERNICKE’S AREA A brain region responsible for the comprehension of language and the production of meaningful speech and writing.
“WHAT” STREAM The
NOTES
PREFACE
1. I have since learned that this observation has resurfaced from time to time, but for obscure reasons isn’t part of mainstream oncology research. See, for example, Havas (1990), Kolmel et al. (1991), or Tang et al. (1991).
INTRODUCTION: NO MERE APE
1. This basic method for studying the brain is how the whole field of behavioral neurology got started back in the nineteenth century. The major difference between then and now is that in those days there was no brain imaging. The doctor had to wait around for a decade or three for the patient to die, then dissect his brain.
2. In contrast to the hobbits, African pigmies, who are also extraordinarily short, are modern humans in every way, from their DNA right on up through their brains, which are the same size as those of all other human groups.
CHAPTER 2 SEEING AND KNOWING
1. Strictly speaking, the fact that octopuses and humans both have complex eyes is probably not an example of true convergent evolution (unlike the wings of birds, bats, and pterosaurs). The same master control genes are at work in “primitive” eyes as in our own. Evolution sometimes reuses genes that have been stored away in the attic.
2. John was originally studied by Glyn Humphreys and Jane Riddoch, who wrote a beautiful monograph about him:
3. Can you see the Dalmatian dog in Figure 2.7?
4. The distinction between the “how” and “what” pathways is based on the pioneering work of Leslie Ungerleider and Mortimer Mishkin working at the National Institutes of Health. Pathways 1 and 2 (“how” and “what”) are clearly defined anatomically. Pathway 3 (dubbed “so what,” or the emotional pathway) is currently considered a functional pathway, as inferred from physiological and brain lesion studies (such as studies on the double dissociation between the Capgras delusion and prosopagnosia; see Chapter 9).
5. Joe LeDoux has discovered there is also a small, ultra-shortcut pathway from the thalamus (and possibly the fusiform gyrus) directly to the amygdala in rats, and quite possibly in primates. But we won’t concern ourselves with that here. The details of neuroanatomy are unfortunately far messier than we would like, but that shouldn’t stop us from looking for overall patterns of functional connectedness, as we’ve been doing.
6. This idea about the Capgras syndrome was proposed independently of us by Hadyn Ellis and Andrew Young. However, they postulate a preserved “how” stream (pathway 1) and combined damage to the two components of the “what” stream (pathways 2 plus 3), whereas we postulate a selective damage to the emotional stream (pathway 3) alone with sparing of pathway 2.
CHAPTER 3 LOUD COLORS AND HOT BABES: SYNESTHESIA
1. Several experiments point to the same conclusion. In our very first paper on synesthesia, published in 2001 in the
2. This basic result—that the
3. In lower, “projection,” synesthetes there are several lines of evidence (in addition to segregation) supporting the low-level perceptual cross-activation model as opposed to the notion that synesthesia is based entirely on high-level associative learning and memories:
(a) In some synesthetes, different parts of a single number or letter are seen as colored differently. (For example, the V part of an
Soon after the