slimy creature.
The man behind these experiments, Eric Kandel, had a motive other than bothering slugs. He wanted to understand the basic mechanism by which learning occurred. What is learning? What changes occur to nerve cells when the brain (or the abdominal ganglion) acquires a new habit or a change in its behaviour? The central nervous system consists of lots of nerve cells, down each of which electrical signals travel; and synapses, which are junctions between nerve cells. When an electrical nerve signal reaches a synapse, it must transfer to a chemical signal, like a train passenger catching a ferry across a sea channel, before resuming its electrical journey. Kandel's attention quickly focused on these synapses between neurons. Learning seems to be a change in their properties.
Thus when a sea slug habituates to a false alarm, the synapse between the receiving, sensory neuron and the neuron that moves the gill is somehow weakened. Conversely, when the sea slug is sensitised to the stimulus, the synapse is strengthened. Gradually and ingeniously, Kandel and his colleagues homed in on a particular molecule in the sea-slug brain which lay at the heart of this weakening or strengthen-ing of the synapses. The molecule is called cyclic A M P .
Kandel and his colleagues discovered a cascade of chemical 2 2 4 G E N O M E
changes all centred around cyclic A M P . Ignoring their names, imagine a string of chemicals called A, B, C and so on: A makes B,
Which activates C,
Which opens a channel called D,
Thus allowing more of E into the cell,
Which prolongs the release of F,
Which is the neurotransmitter that ferries the signal across the synapse to the next neuron.
Now it so happens that C also activates a protein called C R E B
by changing its form. Animals that lack this activated form of C R E B
can still learn things, but cannot remember them for more than an hour or so. This is because C R E B , once activated, starts switching on genes and thus altering the very shape and function of the synapse. The genes thus alerted are called
So simple, in fact, that it is time to meet
Tully then reasoned that if he could knock out the flies' ability M E M O R Y 2 2 5
to learn, he could alter or enhance it as well. By removing the gene for the C R E B protein, he created a fly that could learn, but not remember that it had learnt - the lesson soon faded from its memory.
And he soon developed a strain of fly that learnt so fast that it got the message after a single lesson whereas other flies needed ten lessons to learn to fear a smell that was reliably followed by an electric shock. Tully described these flies as having photographic memories; far from being clever, they over-generalised horribly, like a person who reads too much into the fact that the sun was shining when he had a bicycle accident and refuses thereafter to bicycle on sunny days. (Great human mnemonists, such as the famous Russian Sherashevsky, experience exactly this problem. They cram their heads with so much trivia that they cannot see the wood for the trees. Intelligence requires a judicious mixture of remembering and forgetting. I am often struck by the fact that I easily 'remember' -
i.e., recognise — that I have read a particular piece of text before, or heard a particular radio programme, yet I could not have recited either: the memory is somehow hidden from my consciousness.
Presumably, it is not so hidden in mnemonists' minds.)4
Tully believes that C R E B lies at the heart of learning and memory mechanisms, a sort of master gene that switches on other genes. So the quest to understand learning becomes a genetic quest after all.
Far from escaping from the tyranny of genes by discovering how to learn instead of behave instinctively, we have merely found that the surest way to understand learning is to understand the genes and their products that enable learning to occur.
By now, it will come as no surprise to learn that C R E B is not confined to flies and slugs. Virtually the same gene is present in mice, as well, and mutant mice have already been created by knocking out the mouse
'antisense', or opposite, of the
silences the gene for a while. Likewise, they are super-learners if their
And from mice
to do its job, called
Together with another learning' gene called alpha-integrin, also on chromosome 16, it provides me with a (somewhat weak) excuse for a chapter on learning.