In this kind of diagram, each object is represented by a network that describes relationships between its parts. Then each part, in turn, is further described in terms of relationships between
89
Some persons claim to imagine scenes as though looking at a photograph, whereas other persons report no such vivid experiences. However, some studies appear to show that both are equally good at recalling details of remembered scenes.
90
See, for example, http://www.usd.edu/psyc301/Rensink.htm and http://nivea.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/Mudsplash/Nature_Supp_Inf/Movies/Movie_List.html.
91
This prediction scheme appears in section §6-7 of my 1953 PhD thesis,
92
In Push Singh’s PhD thesis, [ref] two robots actually consider such questions. Also refer to 2004 BT paper.
93
The idea of a panalogy first appeared in
94
I got some of these ideas about ‘trans’ from the early theories of Roger C. Schank, described in
95
96
As suggested in §3-12 we often learn more from failure than from success—because success means you already possessed that skill, whereas failure instructs us to learn something new.
97
See Douglas Lenat,
98
This discussion is adapted from my introduction to
99
From: Alexander R.Luria,
100
Landauer, Thomas K. (1986). “How much do people remember? Some estimates of the quantity of learned information in long-term memory.” Cognitive Science, 10, 477-493. See also Ralph Merkle’s description of this in http://www.merkle.com/humanMemory.html. Furthermore, according to Ronald Rosenfeld, the information in typical text is close to about 6 bits per word. See Rosenfeld, Ronald, “A maximum entropy approach to adaptive statistical language modeling,”